Why People Often Don’t Properly Prepare for Hurricanes

by | January 25, 2018
Category: Uncategorized

A new study looks at the mental biases that prevent people from taking adequate safety measures when a hurricane is on the way.

Over the past few decades, advances in hurricane forecasting, construction codes, and evacuation procedures have helped to save lives and mitigate property damage. Yet economic damage from hurricanes has steadily increased. Researchers from Columbia University, University of Miami, and University of Pennsylvania wanted to find out why. In a paper published last week, the authors outlined the ways in which decision biases interfere with residents’ abilities to protect themselves and minimize property damage from hurricanes.

media.defense.gov

Man boarding up windows as a storm approaches

From following evacuation procedures to ensuring proper insurance coverage to bettering building materials, there are many ways to mitigate a hurricane’s impact on human life and property. The researchers conducted phone surveys as Hurricanes Earl, Irene, Isaac and Sandy threatened their respective targets.  They gathered data on residents’ preparedness and risk perception. They found that decision-making ahead of hurricanes is influenced by a variety of factors, ranging from socioeconomic status to policy incentives to cognitive biases in risk perception.

Though models of human behavior centered on rationality suggest that people should respond to hurricane forecasts by taking steps to reduce their risk of loss, decision science has shown that many factors impede such action. Rather than weighing the relatively low cost of preventive steps, in relation to the high benefit of protection, people are influenced by a variety of factors.

Mental Biases to Preparedness

Humans have a tendency to prioritize short-term benefits at the expense of long-term security. Given this fact, events that seem geographically distant are perceived in a more abstract way, whereas events that seem close seem more concrete. The psychological term for this is “myopia.”  The researchers found evidence of myopia when they surveyed residents about evacuations. People in low-risk situations were more likely to say they would evacuate than the people who were actually in a storm’s path. Myopia also led to poor awareness of the time that the storm was predicted to hit, with residents perceiving more time to prepare than what was really available.

Hurricanes bring several threats—strong wind, rain, and flood hazards. Yet the general public worries mostly about heavy wind, neglecting the other two threats, which have severe impacts. Most people do not understand storm surge, which is often the deadliest impact of a hurricane. This is because our brains generate flawed mental models—the cognitive representations of processes or phenomena that shape our understanding of the world and determine how we make inferences and predictions about future events.  People with inaccurate mental models do not understand evacuation orders well or select the amount of insurance coverage to properly protect them.

People also have trouble understanding probability estimates, especially as they relate to hurricanes. Residents often struggle to differentiate betwee

media.defense.gov

Hurricane Evacuation Route Signs

n the probability of a forecasted event and forecast confidence. Ambiguity, such as magnitude of potential impact, is often confused with uncertainty, such as arrival time or exactly where a hurricane will hit. Words like “large,” “significant,” and “damaging,” foster the ambiguity, while verbal descriptors of likelihood (e.g. “very likely,” “likely,” “unlikely”) are not definite, creating variability in interpretation.

Though one would expect prior experience with a storm to be beneficial to storm preparedness, the researchers have found that prior storm experience can be detrimental to preparing for future storms. People make probability judgements based on how easily they recall similar instances. (This shortcut is called the “availability heuristic.”) The ability to recall, however, is influenced by how long ago the recalled event occurred, how often it happened, and how much emotion the event evoked. Commonly, people who have lived through hurricanes in the past but did not incur property damage underestimate the impact and likelihood of experiencing damage. And people who evacuated in the past for hurricanes that never arrived are far less likely to evacuate again in the future.

Social factors influence how people act. Inevitably and especially in times of crisis, people turn to friends, family, and neighbors to see how they are responding and often act similarly. Seeing others take action is the best predictor of a household’s preparedness. Though respondents did not outright cite friends’ behaviors as influencing their own preparedness choices, there is undoubtedly a correlation between watching your neighbors put up shutters and putting up your own shutters.

Fighting Biases

The researchers offer an integrated set of recommendations to combat these psychological biases against storm preparedness.They seek to streamline information in a way to make it more accessible to target areas. To fight myopia, they suggest updating websites to provide zip codes. By entering a zip code, targeted residents could receive more tailored information and forecasts – such as anticipated wind, storm surge, or evacuation notices – on how a storm will affect their specific neighborhood. This would provide a more tangible forecast for the general public.

Similarly, to address erroneous mental models, the team suggests modifying the presentation of watch, warning, and evacuation information by requiring more specific locations, and tailoring recommended actions accordingly.

Tailoring information would also reduce confusion over the official forecast’s objective probabilities and people’s subjective assessments. In addition, the researchers advise developing new metrics focusing on impacts rather than probabilities, to eliminate some of the miscommunication associated with subjective probabilities. The researchers also suggest developing new metrics focusing on impacts rather than probabilities, eliminating some of the miscommunication associated with subjective probabilities.

Insurance coverage—specifically the lack of incentive to purchase proper insurance, and the lack of public information about the nuances of different types of policies—presents particular challenges to preparedness. The paper recommends taking advantage of the fact that most people rely on the default, by using an opt-out versus an opt-in program. This, for example, would mean homeowners insurance includes an option for flood insurance, but allows residents to opt-out if they desire. Moreover, preparation checklists and evacuation plans should be made prior to the start of hurricane season. Furthermore, policymakers must incentivize residents in high-risk areas to stormproof their homes prior to storm threats. Subsidies, code changes, and insurance-related incentives could easily mitigate some of these issues.

media.defense.gov

Empty supermarket aisle as Sandy approaches New York

Encouraging emergency preparedness is vital to ensuring that residents are ready for the impacts of a hurricane. Understanding individual biases, though not the only challenge to creating preparedness plans, are useful in guiding and developing communication approaches to to alleviate some of the outlined issues, allowing residents to prepare for natural disasters, and make decisions before hurricanes come, not after.

The authors of this study included Kerry Milch, Kenneth Broad, and Ben Orlove from Columbia University’s Center for Research on Environmental Decisions, and Robert Meyer from the University of Pennsylvania. Broad and Orlove also have affiliations with the University of Miami and Columbia’s School for International and Public Affairs, respectively.

Full paper available here.

 

Submit Comment

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *